A Qualitative Analysis of Ableist Microaggressions

Jennifer M. Cook, Melissa D. Deroche, Lee Za Ong

The phenomenon of microaggressions is well established within the counseling literature, particularly as it relates to race, ethnicity, gender, and affectual orientation. However, research related to disability or ableist microaggressions is still in its infancy, so counseling professionals have limited information about experiences of disability and ableist microaggressions. The purpose of this qualitative content analysis was to describe participants’ self-reported experiences with ableist microaggressions. Participants (N = 90) had a diagnosed disability and the majority (91.11%) identified as having two or more nondominant identities beyond their disability. We report two categories and 10 themes. While participants were part of the general population, we position our discussion and implications within the context of professional counseling to increase counseling professionals’ awareness and knowledge so counselors can avoid ableist microaggressions and provide affirmative counseling services to persons with disabilities.

Keywords: disability, ableist microaggressions, professional counseling, nondominant identities, affirmative counseling

Day by day, what you choose, what you think, and what you do is who you become.
—Heraclitus, pre-Socratic philosopher

     Each person is a complex makeup of dominant and nondominant sociocultural identities. Individuals with dominant cultural identities (e.g., able-bodied, White, middle social class) experience societal privilege, have more sociocultural influence, and have unencumbered access to resources. People with nondominant identities, including people with disabilities (PWD), people of color, and people in lower social class, frequently have less influence and experience structural and interpersonal inequities, limitations, and discrimination (Sue & Spanierman, 2020). As such, people with nondominant identities often experience microaggressions. Microaggressions are unintentional or deliberate verbal, nonverbal, and/or environmental messages that convey disapproval, distaste, and condemnation of an individual based on their nondominant identity (Sue et al., 2007).

Professional counselors are aware and knowledgeable that their identity constellation and their
experiences with microaggressions, as well as those of their clients, impact their worldviews, experiences, and—importantly—the counseling relationship (Ratts et al., 2016). While microaggressions associated with several cultural identities have been well-researched within counseling (e.g., race, ethnicity, gender, affectual orientation), others, like ableist microaggressions, have been examined far less frequently (Deroche et al., 2024). The purpose of this article is to describe the microaggression experiences that PWD (N = 90) encounter. Our intention is to increase counseling professionals’ awareness and knowledge about ableist microaggressions so they can examine their beliefs about disability, identify how they may have participated in ableist microaggressions and, ultimately, provide affirmative counseling services to PWD.

Literature Review

Although the term microaggressions was coined by Pierce in the 1970s, it was not until 2007 that it took hold within the allied helping professions (Sue et al., 2007). Initially, the term was used to describe experiences based on race, yet the term has been applied more broadly to the dismissive experiences people with other nondominant identities (e.g., gender, affectual/sexual orientation) encounter (Sue & Spanierman, 2020). In 2010, Keller and Galgay initiated foundational research about the microaggressions that PWD experience. Through their qualitative study, they identified eight microaggression domains experienced by PWD and described their harmful effects on the psychological and emotional well-being of PWD. Those eight domains are: (a) denial of identity, (b) denial of privacy, (c) helplessness, (d) secondary gain, (e) spread effect, (f) patronization, (g) second-class citizenship, and (h) desexualization (i.e., ignoring or avoiding the sexual needs, wants, or desires of PWD). This study marked the beginning of ableist microaggressions research that led scholars not only to naming (e.g., Dávila, 2015) and measuring (e.g., Conover et al., 2017a) specific microaggressions toward PWD, but also describing experiences with ableist microaggressions within specific disability groups (e.g., Coalson et al., 2022; Eisenman et al., 2020) and exploring the impact for specific cultural groups of PWD (e.g., Miller & Smith, 2021).

Before continuing further, it is important for us to explain our use of the term ableist microaggressions, rather than the term disability microaggressions, because it deviates from the typical convention used to name microaggressions (e.g., racial microaggressions, gender microaggressions). While some authors have used the term disability microaggressions (e.g., Dávila, 2015), we believe that this term undercuts and minimizes PWD’s microaggression experiences, as it fails to explicitly communicate that these microaggressions are forms of ableism. Therefore, to validate PWD’s experiences and to align with the disability movement’s philosophy of diversity and social justice, we use the term ableist microaggressions (Perrin, 2019).

The qualitative ableist microaggression studies we reviewed all utilized and endorsed the themes Keller and Galgay (2010) found in their qualitative study, while adding nuance and new information about ableist microaggressions. For instance, Olkin et al.’s (2019) focus group research with women who had both hidden and apparent disabilities (N = 30) supported Keller and Galgay’s eight themes while identifying two others: medical professionals not believing PWD’s symptoms and experiences of having their disability discounted based on appearing young and/or healthy. Similarly, Coalson et al. (2022), who utilized focus groups with adults who stutter (N = 7), endorsed six of Keller and Galgay’s themes and identified participants’ perceptions of microaggressive behaviors (i.e., Exonerated the Listener, Benefit of the Doubt, Focusing on Benefits, and Aggression Viewed as Microaggression) while noting that some participants had minimal or no microaggression experiences.

Although Eisenman et al. (2020) endorsed five of Keller and Galgay’s (2010) themes, they took a different approach to how they analyzed and organized their findings by using Sue et al.’s (2007) microaggression taxonomy. Of note, these researchers were the first to identify and establish microaffirmations within disability microaggressions research. According to Rolón-Dow and Davison (2018) microaffirmations are:

behaviors, verbal remarks or environmental cues experienced by individuals from minoritized racial groups in the course of their everyday lives that affirm their racial identities, acknowledge their racialized realities, resist racism or advance cultural and ideological norms of racial justice. (p. 1)

Like microaggressions, microaffirmations may be intentional or unintentional, but they have a positive rather than a negative impact on people with nondominant racial identities. Eisenman et al. (2020) found all four race-related microaffirmation types identified by Rolón-Dow and Davison (2021)—Microrecognitions, Microvalidations, Microtransformations, and Microprotections—with their sample of people with intellectual disabilities.

Finally, Miller and Smith (2021) conducted individual interviews (N = 25) with undergraduate and graduate students who identified as members of the LGBTQ community with a disability. They, too, found Keller and Galgay’s (2010) domains present in their study and identified eight additional categories. Five categories captured cultural components in addition to disability (i.e., Biphobia, Intersectionality Microaggression, Queer Passing/Disclosure, Racism, and Sexism), while the remaining three were specific to ableist microaggression–focused data: Ableism Avoidance, Faculty Accommodations, and Structural Ableism/Inaccessibility.

The purpose of our study is to add to the burgeoning disability and microaggressions discourse by analyzing participants’ responses to a qualitative prompt offered to them after they completed the Ableist Microaggression Scale (AMS; Conover et al., 2017b). We corroborate prior research findings while adding novel findings that increase professional knowledge about ableist microaggressions and their impact.

Methodology

To ensure compliance with Section 508 of the Rehabilitation Act, the federal law that requires PWD to have access to electronic information equivalent to that available to nondisabled individuals, we utilized digital accessibility tools on the internet platform used for this study (Qualtrics) and recruited PWD to test the accessibility of the study survey and questions. The data analyzed and reported in this article were part of a larger, IRB-approved study (N = 201) in which we investigated participants’ ableist microaggression experiences quantitatively using the AMS (Conover et al., 2017b) to uncover whether participants’ AMS scores were impacted by visibility of disability, type of disability, and their other nondominant identities (Deroche et al., 2024). After participants completed the survey, they were invited to provide a written response to the open-ended question: “What, if any, information do you think would be helpful for us to know about your personal experiences regarding ableist microaggressions?” Ninety participants (44.77% of the overall sample) responded with rich data that warranted analysis and reporting in an independent article. Because the open-ended question occurred after participants completed the AMS, we agreed that the survey likely influenced their responses, so we chose to conduct a content analysis using an a priori codebook grounded in the AMS subscales (Minimization, Denial of Personhood, Otherization, and Helplessness; Conover et al., 2017b), with additional coding categories for data that did not fit the a priori codes (i.e., Fortitude/Resilience/Coping, Contextual Factors, Impact of Microaggressions/Ableism on Mental Health/Wellness, Microaggression Experiences Are Different Depending on Visibility of Disability, Internalized Ableism, and Microaggressions Include Identities Other Than Disability).

Procedure
     Using online data collection via Qualtrics survey, we recruited participants nationally by contacting disability organizations, listservs, social media, and professional contacts who work with organizations that serve PWD. The recruitment included a description of the research; inclusion criteria; and a confidential, anonymized survey link. The survey was Section 508–compliant and optimized to be taken on a computer or mobile device. Data were collected over a 3-month period.

Inclusion Criteria and Participants
     To participate in the study, individuals (a) were at least 18 years of age, (b) had earned a high school diploma or GED, and (c) had a diagnosed disability. Under the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), the term disability is defined within the context of a person’s significant limitations to engage in major life activities. Different agencies and organizations such as the World Health Organization and the U.S. Social Security Administration define disability differently (Patel & Brown, 2017). For this study, we categorized disability as (a) physical disability (i.e., mobility-related disability), (b) sensory disability (i.e., seeing- or hearing-related disability), (c) psychiatric/mental disability (e.g., bipolar disorder, depression, post-traumatic stress disorder), or (d) neurodevelopmental disability (e.g., autism spectrum disorder, learning disability, or ADHD). Participants’ disabilities were apparent/visible (i.e., recognizable by others without the person disclosing they have a disability) or hidden (i.e., others are unlikely to know the individual has a disability, so the person must disclose they have a disability for it to be known), and they could identify with one or more disability categories listed above. Ninety individuals provided usable responses. Table 1 details participant demographics. The bulk of the sample, 84.43%, identified as having two (36.66%), three (26.66%), or four (21.11%) nondominant cultural identities out of the six identities the study targeted, while the rest of the sample comprised individuals who noted six (n = 2; 2.22%), five (n = 4; 4.4%), one (n = 7; 7.77%), or no (n = 1; 1.11%) nondominant identities.

Of note, a higher percentage of participants with hidden or both apparent and hidden disabilities participated in the qualitative portion of the study compared to those who completed only the quantitative portion (45.5% compared to 41.8% and 33.3% compared to 27.4%, respectively). Similarly, there was a lower response rate from participants who earned a high school diploma or GED (5.6%), completed an associate degree or trade school (7.8%), completed some college (7.8%), or earned a doctoral degree (10%). There was an increase in responses from participants who earned a bachelor’s degree (26.7% compared to 21.9% in the quantitative portion) or a master’s degree (42.2% compared to 35.8%, respectively).

Data Analysis
     We analyzed data for this study using MacQueen et al.’s (1998) framework to create a codebook to promote coder consistency. We established six codes, four of which were definitionally congruent with the AMS subscales (i.e., Helplessness, Minimization, Denial of Personhood, and Otherization; Conover et al., 2017a). While we used Conover et al.’s definitions as the foundation, we utilized Keller and Galgay’s (2010) definitions to add additional nuance. The next code, Other Data, was an a priori code reserved for data that did not fit the AMS subscale codes. After completing the pilot, we added a sixth code, Fortitude/Resilience/Coping, to capture data that demonstrated ways in which participants developed strengths, dealt with adversity and microaggressions, and persevered despite their microaggressive experiences. Identifying PWD’s fortitude/resilience/coping abilities is indicative of a strengths-based framework that promotes inclusion, equity, and higher quality of life. Research has shown that resilience in PWD such as improved well-being, higher social role satisfaction, and lower mental health symptoms are correlated with positive psychological and employment outcomes (Ordway et al., 2020; Norwood et al., 2022).  Once this code was established, the Other Data code was used for any data that did not fit the five a priori codes. After the pilot, we added to the codebook definitions for clarity—though no codes were changed. All codes we established had substantial representation in the data and are reported as themes in the results section. The auditor (second author Melissa D. Deroche) gave feedback on the codebook and confirmed the codebook was sound prior to analysis.

Table 1
Demographic Characteristics of Participants (N = 90)

Variable        n          %
Disability Type
Single Type: Physical 21 23.33
Single Type: Sensory 17 18.88
Single Type: Neurodevelopmental 6 6.66
Single Type: Psychiatric/Mental Health 6 6.66
Combination (2): Physical and Psychiatric/Mental Health 8 8.88
Combination (2): Neurodevelopmental and Psychiatric/Mental Health 6 6.66
Combination (2): Sensory and Psychiatric/Mental Health 5 5.55
Combination (2): Sensory and Physical 4 4.44
Combination (2): Neurodevelopmental and Physical 2 2.22
Combination (2): Sensory and Neurodevelopmental 2 2.22
Combination (3): Physical, Psychiatric/Mental Health, Neurodevelopmental 4 4.44
Combination (3): Physical, Sensory, Neurodevelopmental 4 4.44
Combination (3): Sensory, Psychiatric/Mental Health, Neurodevelopmental 2 2.22
Combination (3): Physical, Sensory, Psychiatric/Mental Health 1 1.11
Combination (4): Physical, Sensory, Psychiatric/Mental Health,

Neurodevelopmental

2 2.22
Visibility of Disability
Visible/Apparent 19 21.11
Hidden/Concealed 41 45.55
Both 30 33.33
Biological Sex/Sex Assigned at Birth
Female 74 82.22
Male 16 17.77
Gender Identity
Gender Fluid/Gender Queer 6 6.66
Man 16 17.77
Woman 68 75.55
Affectual/Sexual Orientation
Asexual 2 2.22
Bisexual 9 10.00
Gay 2 2.22
Heterosexual 68 75.55
Lesbian 3 3.33
Pansexual 4 4.44
Queer 1 1.11
Questioning 1 1.11
Racial/Ethnic Identity
African American/Black 4 4.44
Asian or Pacific Islander 3 3.33
Biracial 2 2.22
Euro-American/White 69 76.66
Indigenous 1 1.11
Jewish 5 5.55
Latino/a or Hispanic 3 3.33
Middle Eastern 1 1.11
Multiracial 2 2.22
Religious/Spiritual Identity
Atheist 8 8.88
Catholic 12 13.3
Jewish 4 4.44
Not Religious 1 1.11
Pagan 1 1.11
Protestant 36 40.00
Questioning 2 2.22
Spiritual Not Religious 5 5.55
Unitarian Universalist 2 2.22
Self-Identify in Another Way 19 21.11
Highest Level of Education
High School Diploma or GED 5 5.55
Associate or Trade School Degree 7 7.77
Some College, No Degree 7 7.77
Bachelor’s Degree 24 26.66
Master’s Degree 38 42.22
PhD, EdD, JD, MD, etc. 9 10.00
No Response 1 1.11
Employment Status
Full-Time 40 44.44
Part-Time 16 17.77
Retired 9 10.00
Student 11 12.22
Unemployed 14 15.55
Employment Compared to Training and Skills
Training/Education/Skills are lower than job responsibilities/position 2 2.22
Training/Education/Skills are on par with job responsibilities/position 42 46.66
Training/Education/Skills exceed job responsibilities/position 24 26.66
Not applicable 22 24.44

We began analysis by piloting 10% of the data (n = 9) using the initial codebook (Boyatzis, 1998). Two researchers (first and third authors Jennifer M. Cook and Lee Za Ong) coded data independently and then worked together to reach consensus. Once the pilot analysis was complete, we coded the remaining data and recoded pilot data to ensure they fit the revised coding frame. After all data were coded, we further coded the data that were assigned to Other Data using in vivo codes to establish codes that best captured the data. We identified five codes within Other Data: Contextual Factors, Impact of Microaggressions/Ableism on Mental Health/Wellness, Microaggression Experiences Are Different Depending on Visibility of Disability, Internalized Ableism, and Microaggressions Include Identities Other Than Disability. 

Trustworthiness
     Cook and Ong coded all data independently and then met to reach consensus. Prior to coding commencement, we identified our beliefs and potential biases about the data and discussed how they might impact coding; we continued these conversations throughout analysis. For the pilot coding phase, independent coder agreement prior to consensus was 40%. Independent coder agreement prior to consensus during regular coding was 56%, and 69% for Other Data independent coding. We reached consensus for all coded data through a team meetings consensus process (Boyatzis, 1998). Finally, we utilized an auditor (Deroche). Deroche reviewed all consensus findings during all analysis stages. The coding team met with the auditor to resolve questions and discrepancies, such as a few instances in which data were misassigned to a code.

Research Team
     The research team comprised three cisgender women between the ages of 45 and 55 who are all licensed professional counselors and work as counselor educators. Cook and Deroche identify as White and hold PhDs in counselor education, while Ong holds a PhD in rehabilitation psychology and is Asian American of Chinese descent and an immigrant from Malaysia. Deroche identifies as a person with a disability, Deroche and Ong have worked extensively with PWD, and all three authors have conducted research about PWD. Cook has abundant publications in qualitative research designs related to multicultural counseling. Finally, all three authors have extensive research training and experience in qualitative and quantitative research designs.

Findings

The findings described below are organized into two categories: findings that align with the AMS subscales and unique findings that are independent of the AMS subscales. Themes are listed in their appropriate category with participants’ quotes to illustrate and substantiate each theme (see Table 2). When we provide participant quotes, we refer to them by their randomly assigned participant numbers (e.g., P105, P109).

Table 2
Categories and Themes

Category/Theme      n % of Sample
Category 1: Findings That Align With the AMS Subscales
Minimization 35           38.88
Denial of Personhood 26           28.88
Otherization 17           18.88
Helplessness 16           17.77
Category 2: Unique Findings Independent of the AMS Subscales
Fortitude/Resilience/Coping 27           30.00
Contextual Factors 17           18.88
Impact of Microaggressions/Ableism on Mental Health/Wellness 10             9.00
Microaggression Experiences Are Different Depending on Visibility of Disability 6             6.66
Internalized Ableism 4             4.44
Microaggressions Include Identities Other Than Disability 4             4.44

Note. N = 90.

Category 1: Findings That Align With the AMS Subscales
     Our analysis revealed that the AMS a priori codes fit the study data. As such, the codes were transitioned to themes: Minimization (n = 35), Denial of Personhood (n = 26), Otherization (n = 17), and Helplessness (n = 16). The quotes selected for each theme illustrate the lived experiences of the theme definitions and add context and nuance about the impact of ableist microaggressions.

Minimization
     Conover et al. (2017a) defined Minimization as microaggression experiences demonstrating the belief that PWD are “overstating their impairment or needs” and that “individuals with a disability could be able-bodied if they wanted to be or that they are actually able-bodied” (p. 581). Thirty-five of the 90 participants’ responses (33.33%) indicated instances of Minimization.

For example, P105 described incidents from their formative years that highlight the belief that PWD are, in fact, able-bodied and overstating their impairment:

As a child, children and adults alike would test the limits of my blindness. My piers [sic] would ask me how many fingers they were holding up. And in one instance, teachers lined a hallway with chairs to see if I’d run into them. Spoiler alert, I did.

P109 spoke to their interactions with family that highlight how disbelief about a person’s disability can result in Minimization:

Family is really bad. They still don’t believe me. I was asked (when I couldn’t climb stairs into a restaurant) are you trying to make a point? My visible disability has gotten worse over 40 years. I think because they saw me before I started using a cane, they just won’t believe me.

P158 illuminated a fallacy that can result in Minimization: “Because my disability is invisible people assume I need no help, [and] when I do, they discount my disability. I hear, ‘you don’t look like you have a disability‚’ ‘don’t sell yourself short.’”

Finally, P137 spoke to the blame that underlies Minimization:

On[e] of the most frequent microaggressions encountered living with my particular invisible disability (type 1 diabetes) is the ableist idea that health is entirely a personal responsibility. There is this assumption that whatever problems we face with our health are a direct result of poor choices (dietary, financial) completely ignoring the systematic problems with for-profit health care in this country.

Denial of Personhood
     Denial of Personhood is characterized by PWD being “treated with the assumption that a physical disability indicates decreased mental capacity and therefore, being reduced to one’s physicality” (Conover et al., 2017a, p. 581); such microaggressions can occur “when any aspect of a person’s identity other than disability is ignored or denied” (Keller & Galgay, 2010, p. 249). Twenty-six participants (28.88%) endorsed this theme. For example, P142 described their experiences in the workplace that illustrate the erroneous belief that PWD have diminished mental capacity: “All my life I was pushed out of jobs for not hearing. People would actually tell me, ‘if you can’t hear—how can you do anything’ even though all my performance reviews exceeded expectations.” P123 spoke to a similar sentiment: “[I] am often asked ‘what’s wrong with you?’ ‘how did you get through college?’” Finally, P173 summarized the belief that seemingly underlies Denial of Personhood microaggressions and issued a corrective action:

Disabled doesn’t mean stupid. We can figure out most things for ourselves and if we can’t we know to ask for help. Don’t tell us how to live our lives or say we don’t deserve love, happiness and children. If you don’t know the level of someone’s disability you shouldn’t have the right to judge them about such things.

Otherization
     Seventeen participants (18.88%) described Otherization as part of their narrative responses. Otherization microaggressions are those in which PWD are “treated as abnormal, an oddity, or nonhuman, and imply people with disabilities are or should be outside the natural order” (Conover et al., 2017a, p. 581) and that their “rights to equality are denied” (Keller & Galgay, 2010, p. 249). Participants shared several examples of these types of microaggressions. For instance, P140 shared:

When we (PWD) ask for simple things (e.g., can you turn on the captioning) and people grumble, say they can’t, etc. it just reinforces that we’re not on equal footing and at least for me it eats away a little bit every time.

P185 indicated another manifestation of Otherization: “As a deaf person, I get frustrated when whoever I’m talking to stops listening when someone else (non-deaf person) speaks verbally, leaving me mid-sentence.” P108 shared that they have been “prayed for in public without asking,” while P106 expressed, “I hate when people compliment me on how well I push my chair or say I must have super strong arms. I just have normal arms not athletic looking or anything.” 

Helplessness
     Helplessness microaggressions are those in which PWD are “treated as if they are incapable, useless, dependent, or broken, and imply they are unable to perform any activity without assistance” (Conover et al., 2017a, p. 581). Sixteen participants (17.77%) described Helplessness microaggressions. For P174, the most common Helplessness microaggression they experience is when “people speak to the person I am with instead of to me. Drives me crazy! Worse is when the person I’m with answers for me.” P126 corroborated the “devastating” nature of when “people make decisions for you.” P129 shared that, “As a person with an invisible disability, I most often encounter microaggressions in the form of unsolicited advice when I disclose my disability.” Similarly, P134 noted:

Although my disability is not apparent, if people know about it, they often just act on my behalf without asking me for input or feedback. That is very frustrating and often does not change even if I bring it up to the individual who does it.

This final quote from P134 is powerful because it, like P174’s experience, demonstrates how people without disabilities participate in perpetuating ableism even when they were not the ones who initiated it.

Category 2: Unique Findings Independent of the AMS Subscales
     As we indicated earlier, we separated data that did not fit into AMS codes and coded them using in vivo codes. This analysis resulted in six novel themes (i.e., Fortitude/Resilience/Coping, Contextual Factors, Impact of Microaggressions/Ableism on Mental Health/Wellness, Microaggression Experiences Are Different Depending on Visibility of Disability, Internalized Ableism, and Microaggressions Include Identities Other Than Disability) that are independent from the AMS-driven themes discussed in the prior section, yet are interrelated because they add unique insights and helpful context for understanding ableist microaggressions within the lived experiences of PWD.

Fortitude/Resilience/Coping
     We defined Fortitude/Resilience/Coping as ways in which participants have developed strength, dealt with adversity/microaggressions, and persevered despite their microaggressive experiences. Thirty percent (n = 27) of participants disclosed a wide range of attitudinal and experiential tactics related to this theme. P103 shared, “I maintain what I call a healthy sense of humor about my own body and being disabled,” while P145 demonstrated a sense of humor as they shared how they cope:

I just have to remind them and myself that my brain works differently and that I am just as competent as anyone else. I have learned not to beat myself up when I forget something or can’t get my paperwork done correctly for the tenth time. (I really hate paperwork.)

Participants 138 and 127 both spoke directly to the role knowledge plays. P138 shared:

I want to put out there that knowledge & understanding are power. Knowing & understanding your rights as a person with a disability as well as knowing & understanding your unique experience with your own disability (to the best of your ability) is key to making forward strides in environments that can often times feel ableist.

P127 spoke to knowledge, too, with their belief that “most microaggressions stem from a lack of education. I am often the first person they have met with a disability and the experience makes them uncomfortable.”

Finally, P187 spoke to the power of their resilience and its impact on their life, experiences which they draw from to help others:

I’ve been physically and emotionally abused my entire life, until I took control and stopped it. I’m middle aged and it took me 40 years to forgive everything that I’ve . . . had to endure. Never from my family, or close friends, but it’s been a difficult life, and now I’m all ok with it and try to help others with disabilities that are having a hard time.

Contextual Factors
     Seventeen participants (18.88%) described Contextual Factors, which are data that depict relational, situational, or environmental elements that impact participants’ experiences of ableist microaggressions.

P110 shared thatmicroaggressions can be hard to label because they can vary based on the relationship you have with the person.” P175 added: “Most times the microaggression I receive are by people when they don’t know me, or first meet me, as opposed to get to know me better.” P162 spoke to additional situational/relational nuances: “I have very different experiences depending on what assistive technology I’m using in a given space (basically to what degree I pass as able-bodied) and how people know me.”

P163 spoke to relational roles as well as environmental context: “The attitudes about me are distinctly divided between the power structures. A case manager, medical doctor, neighbor or family member will certainly show their attitude differently. The same goes for academic settings [versus] job placement.” For P152, “The worst comments have come from mental health therapists [who] are medical professionals who should be the most compassionate towards their patients.”

P117 and P131 both identified situational differences they have noticed. P117 shared, “I find that people have treated me differently at different ages and stages in my life, particularly when I was raising three children as a divorced mom.” P131 identified their work environment as positive: “I work in the field of vocational rehabilitation so [I] interact with more people who have a more nuanced understanding of disability than the general population.” However, P165 offered an alternate view, noting that “many microaggressions are more insidious or come from within the disabled community.”

Impact of Microaggressions/Ableism on Mental Health/Wellness
     Ten participants (9%) expressed how microaggressions and ableism experiences have impacted their mental health and wellness. P172 stated, “I struggle with my mental wellness and I have been hospitalized for severe depression that manifests from a combination of my disability and situations that are overwhelming.” P157 expressed a similar combination effect of having a disability and being “ostracized” by others: “The combination is very heavy on my heart and leaves me feeling incredibly alone.”

P159 expressed feeling “pathetic and weak. Sometimes I feel useless and disgraced. Most of the time I feel dumb and stupid.” P103 added additional impacts while acknowledging the differences between their experiences and those of their colleagues of color: “None of these [microaggressions] were overt, but all contributed to stress and frustration and generalized anxiety. I have seen much worse with coworkers of color and disabled Black and Brown folks in my community.”

P126 admitted that completing the study survey “evoked difficult memories.” Additionally, this participant described the turmoil and cognitive dissonance they experience:

I’m reminded taking this survey of the inner conflict with identifying as disabled. Is my disability qualifying enough, will I be rejected? I felt hints of defensiveness emerge, like imposter syndrome. I also recognize that I desire to be abled and that keeps the conflict churning.

Microaggression Experiences Are Different Depending on Visibility of Disability
     Six participants (6.6%) spoke to how individuals with hidden disabilities experience microaggressions differently than individuals with visible/apparent disabilities. P141 asserted that “because my disabilities are hidden, I don’t hear many microaggressions regarding me,” and P183 corroborated that microaggressions are “different the more severe and obvious the disabilities are.”

P146 suggested that “invisible disabilities offer up a whole different category of microaggressions than those with visible disabilities,” and P151 added that “hidden disabilities is [sic] a double edged sword,” highlighting both the privilege and the dismissiveness hidden disabilities can bring. P150 emphasized the privilege of others not knowing about their disability: “In some ways, this benefits me because I’m not associated with the stigma of a disability.”

Internalized Ableism
     A small number of participants (n = 4) expressed comments that were consistent with Internalized Ableism. Internalized Ableism includes believing the stereotypes, myths, and misconceptions about PWD, such as the notion that all disabilities are visible and that PWD cannot live independently, and it can manifest as beliefs about their own disability or others’ disabilities. One manifestation of Internalized Ableism is when a PWD expresses that another’s disability is not real or true compared to their own disability. For example, P112 stated:Every time I go out I have great difficulty finding available accessible parking. I watch & people using the spots are walking/functioning just fine. Sick of hearing about ‘hidden disability.’ I think the majority are inconsiderate lazy people.”

Another manifestation of Internalized Ableism can be when PWD deny the existence of ableist microaggressions. P183 shared:

I don’t think that most people have microaggressions toward PWD. Maybe that’s different the more severe and obvious the disabilities are. It tends to be older people like 60s or 70s that treat me differently period it seems like the younger generation just sees most of us as people not disabled people. And I also think the term ableist separates PWD and people without. If we don’t want to be labeled, we shouldn’t label them.

Microaggressions Include Identities Other Than Disability
     For this final theme, four participants (4.44%) spoke to the complexity related to microaggressions when a PWD has additional nondominant cultural identities. P167 expressed the compounding effect: “I have multiple minoritized identities—the intersection leads to more biases.” P161 articulated the inherent confusion when one has multiple nondominant identities: “I do not know whether I am treated in the ways I indicated because of my disabilities or because I am a person of color.” These quotes highlight the inherent increase and subsequent impact on PWD who have more than one nondominant cultural identity.

Discussion

The purpose of our analysis was to illuminate participants’ lived experiences with ableist microaggressions that were important to them. We revealed contextual information about participants’ experiences that aligned with the AMS subscales (i.e., Minimization, Denial of Personhood, Otherization, and Helplessness). Although prior qualitative ableist microaggression studies (e.g., Coalson et al., 2022; Eisenman et al., 2020; Olkin et al., 2019) grounded their research in Keller and Galgay’s (2010) eight categories rather than in Conover et al.’s (2017a) four subscales, it is fair to say that our findings substantiate other researchers’ findings because Conover et al.’s four subscales were devised based on Keller and Galgay’s findings.

While the corroboration of prior research findings based on the AMS subscales is illustrative and essential, the crucial findings from this study lie in the unique themes that arose from the in vivo coding process (i.e., Fortitude/Resilience/Coping, Contextual Factors, Impact of Microaggressions/Ableism on Mental Health/Wellness, Microaggression Experiences Are Different Depending on Visibility of Disability, Internalized Ableism, and Microaggressions Include Identities Other Than Disability). These themes introduce both novel and less-explored aspects of disability and of ableist microaggressions.

Fortitude/Resilience/Coping is a unique theme. Participants described how they became stronger and persevered despite microaggressive experiences. Eisenman et al. (2020) were the first to identify microaffirmations within ableist microaggressions research and Coalson et al. (2022) found that their participants perceived benefits that came from microaggressive experiences; both are important contributions. However, both instances of seeming positives related to ableist microaggressions in these studies are framed within the context of how others acted toward PWD rather than the autonomous choices and personal development of the person with the disability in the face of adversity. Our findings demonstrate PWD’s abilities—both innate qualities and learned skills—that rendered life-giving fortitude, resilience, and coping in which they are personally empowered and persevere despite external stimuli; they are not dependent upon whether others act appropriately. This is a key finding for counselors because they have the ability to create a therapeutic environment in which PWD can process, develop, and refine their fortitude, resilience, and coping further, acknowledging that PWD have these skills already.

Unsurprisingly, some participants spoke to the impact of ableist microaggressions and ableism on their mental health and wellness; these impacts included depression, loneliness, stress, frustration, and feeling “pathetic and weak.” What was surprising is that only 9% of the sample spoke to this impact directly, given how well-documented the harmful mental health effects of microaggressions are (Sue & Spanierman, 2020). This seeming underrepresentation of mental health ramifications amongst participants led us to wonder, based on the high percentage of participants (30%) who endorsed Fortitude/Resilience/Coping, whether this specific sample had a uniquely high ability to cope with adversity as compared to the overall disability population or if it is possible that ableist microaggression experiences have begun to decrease. While we are unable to answer these questions directly as part of this study, we posit three considerations: (a) microaggressions continue to have a negative effect on some PWD and need to be screened for and attended to within the counseling process; (b) screening for and helping clients with disabilities name, develop, or refine coping, fortitude, and resilience can prove beneficial; and (c) it is worthwhile to continue to work to reduce microaggressive behaviors in every way possible.

Although we had an independent theme in which participants indicated the differences between apparent and hidden disabilities, the participant quotes within every theme illustrate these differences as well. For instance, within the Minimization theme, P137 highlighted that those with hidden disabilities may be told that “personal responsibility” is the cause of their disability, while P105 and P109 spoke to having to “prove” their apparent disability to others, including family. Having to prove one’s disability or not being believed tracks with several other researchers’ findings including Olkin et al. (2019), who found that medical professionals did not believe PWD’s symptoms and experiences. The Helplessness theme revealed differences such as P129 receiving unsolicited advice once people learn of their hidden disability; however, this theme revealed similarities, too. Participants with both apparent and hidden disabilities experienced others acting on their behalf without their consent.

The Microaggression Experiences Are Different Depending on Visibility of Disability theme may explain why a higher percentage of participants with hidden disabilities or those who have both hidden and apparent disabilities participated in the qualitative portion of the study than those with apparent disabilities, which was the higher percentage in the quantitative part of the study. By definition, microaggressions can leave those who experience them questioning whether what they experienced was real, and this could be compounded when PWD have hidden disabilities; these participants may have needed to express their experiences more than those with apparent disabilities. While our data demonstrate that having a hidden disability may be a protective factor from experiencing ableist microaggressions, their disability experience often can be overlooked or ignored, resulting in a form of minimization that is both congruent with and distinct from the Minimization subscale definition.

Participants made a case for how Contextual Factors, defined as relational, situational, and/or environmental components, impact microaggression experiences. Implicitly, several authors spoke to what we have named as Contextual Factors (e.g., Coalson et al., 2022; Eisenman et al., 2020; Miller & Smith, 2021), yet the specificity and nuance participants provided in this study warranted a distinct theme. Relationally, participants noted that whether the perpetrator knew them and if there was a relational power differential between them and the perpetrator (e.g., doctors or counselors vs. family member or neighbor) makes a difference. Damningly, P152 stated that “the worst comments” they have received “have come from mental health therapists.” Participants noted, too, that work environments, life stage, the type of assistive technology they are using at the time, and being part of the disability community can all be impactful in both affirming and deleterious ways. It is imperative that counselors assess and understand thoroughly each client’s specific contextual factors so they can identify ways in which clients have internal and external resources and support, as well as areas in which they may want strategies, support, resources, and, potentially, advocacy intervention.

A small number of participants (n = 4) spoke to Internalized Ableism. Although this was a less robust theme, it was important to report because it adds to professional knowledge about what some clients with disabilities might believe and express during counseling sessions. We defined Internalized Ableism as participants expressing stereotypes, myths, and misconceptions about PWD that can manifest as beliefs about their own disability or the disabilities of others. One participant expressed disdain for hidden disabilities and expressed disbelief about others’ needs to use parking for disabled persons, while another participant questioned whether most PWD experience ableist microaggressions. While our study findings are not congruent with these statements, counselors must take clients’ expressions seriously, work to understand how clients have developed these beliefs, and seek to understand their impact on the client who is stating them.

Finally, four participants indicated that Microaggressions Include Identities Other Than Disability. Given the high percentage of the sample that had multiple nondominant identities, it is curious that so few participants spoke to this phenomenon. However, we theorize that this may have to do with identity salience (Hunt et al., 2006) and the fact that this was a study about ableist microaggressions. For the participants who spoke to this theme, the important features they reported were the compounding effect of microaggressions when one has multiple nondominant identities and the inherent confusion that results from microaggressive experiences, particularly when one has multiple nondominant identities. Again, counselors must screen for and be prepared to address the complexity and the impact of ableist microaggressions based on each client’s unique identities and experiences.

Implications for Practice
     The study findings illustrate the ubiquitous, troubling, and impactful nature of ableist microaggressions. These findings expose many counselors, supervisors, and educators to a world they may not know well or at all, while for others, these findings validate experiences they know all too well personally and professionally. We began this article with a quote from the pre-Socratic philosopher Heraclitus: “Day by day, what you choose, what you think, and what you do is who you become.” This quote captures the charge we are issuing to counseling professionals: It is time to take action to become counseling professionals who think as, act as, and are disability-affirming professionals. The task at hand is for each counseling professional to decide what steps to take next to strengthen their disability-affirming identity based on their current awareness, knowledge, and skill level, as well as how they can enact their disability-affirming identity based on their professional roles.

Fundamentally, disability-affirming professionals validate, support, encourage, and advocate for and with PWD consistently throughout their professional activities. For many, this begins with developing their awareness and knowledge, followed later by their skills. Based on the findings presented in this article, we suggest counseling professionals engage in self-reflexivity by examining the ways in which they have unwittingly adopted the dominant discourses about disability, what they believe about the abilities and lives of PWD, how they understand disability within the context of other nondominant identities, and the ways in which they have participated in perpetuating ableist microaggressions. Without engaging in disability self-awareness development, professionals risk conveying ableist microaggressions to clients that can result in early termination, impede the therapeutic relationship, and/or inflict additional psychological harm (Sue & Spanierman, 2020). For example, counselors may assume that clients with disabilities have diminished social–emotional learning skills compared to clients without disabilities and initiate formalized assessment based on this assumption. While counselors should be attuned to all clients’ social–emotional skills, it can be damaging to PWD’s sense of self and the counseling relationship to assume their social–emotional learning skills are deficient rather than assessing how environments are not conducive to PWD’s social–emotional needs (Lindsay et al., 2023).

Counselors’ self-reflexive process is meant to foster self-awareness; to better equip counselors to recognize ableist microaggressions in clients’ stories when they occur in personal, training, and professional environments; and for them to avoid unintentionally communicating ableist microaggressions in their practice. To start this process, we encourage counselors to question whether any of the study findings rang true, whether as someone who has experienced ableist microaggressions or as one who has perpetrated them, and to ascertain whether their attitudes and beliefs about PWD differ based on the visibility of disability. Additionally, we proffer that counselors who engage in self-reflective activities, such as the ones mentioned above, and those who learn more about PWD’s lives and experiences are more apt to create a plan to work through any negative attitudes or biases they have and, in turn, refine their skills so they are more disability-affirming in their practice. Counselors who engage in these processes will benefit those they serve, whether clients, students, or supervisees.

This study represents only a slice of the microaggression experiences of PWD. We concur with Rivas and Hill (2023) that counselors must adopt an evolving commitment to develop disability counseling effectiveness. Ways that counselors can take steps toward developing their disability-affirmative counselor identity and effectiveness include familiarizing themselves with and applying the American Rehabilitation Counseling Association (ARCA) disability competencies (Chapin et al., 2018); reading additional studies (e.g., Olkin et al., 2019; Peters et al., 2017); listening to podcasts (e.g., Swenor & Reed, n.d.); reading blogs and books (e.g., Heumann & Joiner, 2020); and watching shows and movies that highlight PWD’s experiences, microaggressive and otherwise—PWD are telling their stories and want others to learn from them.

Within the relational context, no matter one’s professional roles, it is important to be prepared to attend to the interaction of identity constellations within professional relationships and the power dynamics that are present (Ratts et al., 2016). Broaching these topics initially, including ability status and similarities and differences with our experiences, is a helpful start; however, this is the beginning of the process, not the entire process. Accordingly, clinicians must continually assess PWD’s contextual factors and their impact, lived experiences of their multiple identities, resilience, fortitude, and coping skills. To do so, clinicians must first create space for clients to process their microaggression experiences through actively listening to their stories; allowing PWD to openly express their frustrations, anger, or other emotions; and validating their experiences using advanced empathy. In other words, it is critical not to dismiss such topics nor unilaterally make them the presenting problem—balance is needed to attend to microaggression experiences appropriately. Essentially, counselors need to guide clients to discern the impact and to identify what they need rather than doing it for them, and to be ready, willing, and able to advocate with and on behalf of clients. All advocacy actions must be discussed with clients so as to center their autonomy.

Clients’ resiliencies and strengths must be fostered unceasingly. It is not uncommon for clients who have experienced ableist microaggressions to feel diminished and worthless and to question their purpose. Counselors must prioritize assisting clients in naming their strengths and telling stories about how they have developed resiliencies, and they must encourage clients to draw on both when facing adversity—particularly ableist microaggressions. While the goal is to eradicate ableist microaggressions, we must reinforce with clients that they are armed with tools to safeguard against ableist microaggressions’ impact and that they can seek trusted support when they need it.

As we move forward into the future as disability-affirming counseling professionals, counselor educators and supervisors have a specific charge to include disability status and disability/ableist microaggressions as part of their professional endeavors when working with students and supervisees. For many, the aforementioned recommendations likely apply because they, too, did not receive education about disability and disability microaggressions (Deroche et al., 2020). This is a setback, but not a limitation. Counselor educators and supervisors are continual learners who seek additional awareness, knowledge, skills, and advocacy actions to positively impact their work with counselors-in-training. Webinars, disability-specific conference sessions, and engaging with community disability organizations are helpful ways to start, and we recommend counselor educators and supervisors engage in the same self-examination strategies mentioned above to begin combating any biases they may hold about PWD. More specifically, counselor educators and supervisors can introduce and teach the ARCA disability competencies to trainees and supervisees, deliberately integrate self-exploration activities regarding disability into coursework, direct trainees and supervisees to inquire about ability status in intake and assessment procedures, and use cultural broaching behaviors to model appropriate use with clients (Deroche et al., 2020).

Limitations and Future Research

There are important limitations to consider to contextualize the study findings. The data used in this analysis were the result of one open-ended prompt as part of a larger quantitative study. Although participants offered robust and illustrative responses, it is a significant limitation that no follow-up questions were asked. Additionally, because the study utilized the AMS (Conover et al., 2017b), we analyzed data using the AMS subscales. While this was an appropriate choice given the context, it limited our ability to compare our findings with other qualitative studies that used Keller and Galgay (2010) to explain their findings.

We recommend that future research investigates the unique themes from this study in more detail to ascertain whether they are applicable to the larger PWD population. We suggest that focus groups combined with individual interviews may help to tease out nuances and could potentially lead to developing theory related to ableist microaggressions and best practices that will support PWD. Finally, we propose that more in-depth intersectionality research would benefit PWD and the professionals who serve them. The confounding nature of microaggressions combined with individuals’ unique identity compositions that often include both nondominant and dominant identities can make this type of research challenging, yet both are the reality for many PWD and this research is therefore needed.

Conclusion

Ableist microaggressions are ubiquitous and damaging to PWD. Through our analysis, we found that participants’ experiences corroborated prior researchers’ findings related to established ableist microaggression categories and added new knowledge by introducing six novel themes. We envision a disability-affirmative counseling profession and offered concrete recommendations for clinicians, supervisors, and counselor educators. Together, we can create a reality in which all PWD who seek counseling services will experience relief, validation, and empowerment as we work to create a society that provides access to all.

 

Conflict of Interest and Funding Disclosure
The authors reported no conflict of interest
or funding contributions for the development
of this manuscript.

 

References

Boyatzis, R. E. (1998). Transforming qualitative information. SAGE.

Chapin, M., McCarthy, H., Shaw, L., Bradham-Cousar, M., Chapman, R., Nosek, M., Peterson, S., Yilmaz, Z., & Ysasi, N. (2018). Disability-related counseling competencies. American Rehabilitation Counseling Association (ARCA) Task Force on Competencies for Counseling Persons with Disabilities. https://arcaweb.org/wp-content/uploads/2023/12/ARCA-Disability-Related-Counseling-Competencies.pdf

Coalson, G. A., Crawford, A., Treleaven, S. B., Byrd, C. T., Davis, L., Dang, L., Edgerly, J., & Turk, A. (2022). Microaggression and the adult stuttering experience. Journal of Communication Disorders, 95, 106180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2021.106180

Conover, K. J., Israel, T., & Nylund-Gibson, K. (2017a). Development and validation of the Ableist Microaggressions Scale. The Counseling Psychologist, 45(4), 570–599. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000017715317

Conover, K. J., Israel, T., & Nylund-Gibson, K. (2017b). Ableist Microaggressions Scale (AMS) [Database record]. APA PsycTESTS. https://doi.org/10.1037/t64519-000

Dávila, B. (2015). Critical race theory, disability microaggressions, and Latina/o student experiences in special education. Race, Ethnicity, and Education, 18(4), 443–468. https://doi.org/10.1080/13613324.2014.885422

Deroche, M. D., Herlihy, B., & Lyons, M. L. (2020). Counselor trainee self-perceived disability competence: Implications for training. Counselor Education and Supervision, 59(3), 187–199. https://doi.org/10.1002/ceas.12183

Deroche, M. D., Ong, L. Z., & Cook, J. M. (2024). Ableist microaggressions, disability characteristics, and nondominant identities. The Professional Counselor, 13(4), 404–417. https://doi.org/10.15241/mdd.13.4.404

Eisenman, L. T., Rolón-Dow, R., Freedman, B., Davison, A., & Yates, N. (2020). “Disabled or not, people just want to feel welcome”: Stories of microaggressions and microaffirmations from college students with intellectual disability. Critical Education, 11(17), 1–21.

Heumann, J., & Joiner, K. (2020). Being Heumann: An unrepentant memoir of a disability rights activist. Beacon Press.

Hunt, B., Matthews, C., Milsom, A., & Lammel, J. A. (2006). Lesbians with physical disabilities: A qualitative study of their experiences with counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development, 84(2), 163–173. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2006.tb00392.x

Keller, R. M., & Galgay, C. E. (2010). Microaggressive experiences of people with disabilities. In D. W. Sue (Ed.), Microaggressions and marginality: Manifestation, dynamics, and impact (pp. 241–268). Wiley.

Lindsay, S., Fuentes, K., Ragunathan, S., Lamaj, L., & Dyson, J. (2023). Ableism within health care professions: A systematic review of the experiences and impact of discrimination against health care providers with disabilities. Disability and Rehabilitation, 45(17), 2715–2731. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2022.2107086

MacQueen, K. M., McLellan, E., Kay, K., & Milstein, B. (1998). Codebook development for team-based qualitative analysis. Cultural Anthropology Methods, 10(2), 31–36. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X980100020301

Miller, R. A., & Smith, A. C. (2021). Microaggressions experienced by LGBTQ students with disabilities. Journal of Student Affairs Research and Practice, 58(5), 491–506. https://doi.org/10.1080/19496591.2020.1835669

Norwood, M. F., Lakhani, A., Hedderman, B., & Kendall, E. (2022). Does being psychologically resilient assist in optimising physical outcomes from a spinal cord injury? Findings from a systematic scoping review. Disability and Rehabilitation, 44(20), 6082–6093. https://doi.org/10.1080/09638288.2021.1952320

Olkin, R., Hayward, H., Abbene, M. S., & VanHeel, G. (2019). The experiences of microaggressions against women with visible and invisible disabilities. Journal of Social Issues, 75(3), 757–785. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12342

Ordway, A. R., Johnson, K. L., Amtmann, D., Bocell, F. D., Jensen, M. P., & Molton, I. R. (2020). The relationship between resilience, self-efficacy, and employment in people with physical disabilities. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 63(4), 195–205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0034355219886660

Patel, D. R., & Brown, K. A. (2017). An overview of the conceptual framework and definitions of disability. International Journal of Child Health and Human Development, 10(3), 247–252.

Perrin, P. B. (2019). Diversity and social justice in disability: The heart and soul of rehabilitation psychology. Rehabilitation Psychology, 64(2), 105–110. https://doi.org/10.1037/rep0000278

Peters, H. J., Schwenk, H. N., Ahlstrom, Z. R., & Mclalwain, L. N. (2017). Microaggressions: The experience of individuals with mental illness. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 30(1), 86–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2016.1164666

Ratts, M. J., Singh, A. A., Nassar-McMillan, S., Butler, S. K., & McCullough, J. R. (2016). Multicultural and social justice counseling competencies: Guidelines for the counseling profession. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 44(1), 28–48. https://doi.org/10.1002/jmcd.12035

Rivas, M., & Hill, N. R. (2023). A grounded theory of counselors’ post-graduation development of disability counseling effectiveness. Journal of Counselor Preparation and Supervision, 17(1). https://digitalcommons.sacredheart.edu/jcps/vol17/iss1/7

Rolón-Dow, R., & Davison, A. (2018). Racial microaffirmations: Learning from student stories of moments that matter. Diversity Discourse, 1(4), 1–9. https://udspace.udel.edu/server/api/core/bitstreams/4230ddcd-b196-4ee2-a5bf-18120b2b1f24/content

Rolón-Dow, R., & Davison, A. (2021). Theorizing racial microaffirmations: A critical race/LatCrit approach. Race Ethnicity and Education, 24(2), 245–261. https://doi.org/10.1080/13613324.2020.1798381

Sue, D. W., Capodilupo, C. M., Torino, G. C., Bucceri, J. M., Holder, A. M. B., Nadal, K. L., & Esquilin, M. (2007). Racial microaggressions in everyday life: Implications for clinical practice. American Psychologist, 62(4), 271–286. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.4.271

Sue, D. W., & Spanierman, L. (2020). Microaggressions in everyday life (2nd ed.). Wiley.

Swenor, B., & Reed, N. (Hosts). (n.d.). Included: The disability equity podcast [Audio podcast]. Johns Hopkins University Disability Health Research Center. https://disabilityhealth.jhu.edu/included

Jennifer M. Cook, PhD, NCC, ACS, LPC, is an associate professor at the University of Texas at San Antonio. Melissa D. Deroche, PhD, NCC, ACS, LPC-S, is an assistant professor at Tarleton State University. Lee Za Ong, PhD, LPC, CRC, is an assistant professor at Marquette University. Correspondence may be addressed to Jennifer M. Cook, University of Texas at San Antonio, Department of Counseling, 501 W. Cesar E. Chavez Blvd, San Antonio, TX 78207, jennifer.cook@utsa.edu.

Ableist Microaggressions, Disability Characteristics, and Nondominant Identities

Melissa D. Deroche, Lee Za Ong, Jennifer M. Cook

Microaggressions are commonplace overt and covert forms of discrimination that convey disparaging messages to individuals who hold marginalized identities, including people with disabilities (PWD). Although PWD are a prevalent nondominant identity group in the United States, little is known about the occurrence of ableist microaggressions in this culturally diverse community, including how ableist microaggressions are experienced based on disability characteristics and other nondominant identities. A sample consisting of 201 PWD completed an electronic survey that examined the occurrence of ableist microaggressions based on visibility of disability, type of disability, and nondominant sociocultural identities. We found that PWD have a moderate level of lifetime occurrences with ableist microaggressions, the visibility of disability impacts the occurrence of ableist microaggressions and the types of ableist microaggressions experienced, and sociocultural identities may predict minimization types of ableist microaggressions.

Keywords: microaggressions, disability, nondominant identities, ableist, visibility

People with disabilities (PWD), similar to other minoritized groups, encounter stereotypes, stigma, prejudice, and discrimination. Stereotypes are generalized beliefs about a group with shared characteristics that can result in prejudice (Sue & Spanierman, 2022). Discrimination occurs when individuals or systems act on prejudices by limiting or restricting access to opportunities, resources, and services (Olkin et al., 2019). Discrimination results in ableism when it is perpetrated against PWD based on their disability status. Microaggressions are one form of discrimination and are defined as intentional or unintentional verbal, non-verbal, and/or environmental slights that convey disparaging messages to individuals based solely on their marginalized group membership status (Sue & Spanierman, 2022).

In recent decades, scholars have examined the occurrence of microaggressions with individuals who hold several different minority group identities and found that microaggressions are present in everyday life for minoritized individuals, negatively impacting their mental health (Capodilupo et al., 2010; Keller & Galgay, 2010; Nadal et al., 2014). Although there are a significant number of studies addressing microaggressions associated with race and ethnicity (Forrest-Bank & Cuellar, 2018; Nadal et al., 2014) and gender and sexual identity (Capodilupo et al., 2010), research regarding microaggressions experienced by PWD is only in its infancy.

In their foundational work, Keller and Galgay (2010) qualitatively explored the existence of microaggressions directed at PWD. Using two focus groups (N = 12) with PWD who had a range of visible and hidden disabilities, they identified eight microaggression domains experienced by PWD: (a) denial of identity, (b) denial of privacy, (c) helplessness, (d) secondary gain, (e) spread effect, (f) patronization, (g) second-class citizen, and (h) desexualization. Their work spurred both qualitative and quantitative studies focused on the ableist microaggression experiences within the general disability community (Kattari, 2020; Lett et al., 2020) and specific disability communities, including people with mental illness (Gonzales et al., 2015), physical disabilities (Conover et al., 2017a), and intellectual disabilities (Eisenman et al., 2020). Also, researchers have examined ableist microaggressions within specific settings such as schools (Dávila, 2015), the workplace (Lee et al., 2019), and higher education (Lett et al., 2020). Collectively, these results supported the initial eight ableist microaggression domains Keller and Galgay identified and expanded how they were understood.

The multidimensionality of disability has led scholars to examine the influence of disability characteristics—including visibility, severity, type, and age of onset of disability—on the occurrence of ableist microaggressions. Visible or apparent disabilities are defined primarily by the use of an adaptive aid or accommodation such as a cane, crutches, service animal, hearing aid, or wheelchair that results in others immediately labeling the person as disabled; whereas people with hidden, invisible, or non-apparent disabilities are likely to pass as nondisabled and must navigate how, when, and to whom to disclose their disability status. Using two different ableist microaggression scales with relatively large samples of PWD, both Conover et al. (2017a) and Kattari (2020) reported that level of visibility of disability impacts the occurrence of ableist microaggressions, with individuals with visible disabilities consistently experiencing more ableist microaggressions than their counterparts. Further examination of these differences resulted in Andreou et al. (2021) reporting that people with visible disabilities encountered more helplessness- and otherization-related ableist microaggressions, while persons with hidden disabilities encountered more minimization-type ableist microaggressions. In contrast, the impact of severity, age of onset, and type of disability on the occurrence of ableist microaggressions have yielded mixed findings, suggesting more research is warranted. For instance, Conover et al. (2017a) found that people with severe and early onset of disability (i.e., 0–40 years) reported greater lifetime experiences of ableist microaggressions, while Andreou et al. (2021) noted no differences based on severity or age of onset. With respect to type of disability, Conover et al. (2017a) reported no differences, while Andreou et al. (2021) found overall differences in the occurrence of ableist microaggressions, as well as type of ableist microaggressions, experienced by people with different types of disabilities.

Although some scholars have considered PWD’s intersecting cultural identities in their research studies, including sexual minorities (e.g., Conover & Israel, 2019; Hunt et al., 2006), gender identities (measured as male and female; Conover et al., 2017a), women with visible and invisible disabilities (Olkin et al., 2019), and racial or ethnic minorities (Conover et al., 2017a; Dávila, 2015), few studies have added to our understanding of ableist microaggressions across intersecting sociocultural identities. For example, Conover and Israel (2019) found that PWD who were also sexual minorities encountered ableist microaggressions in sexual minority communities, while Conover et al. (2017a) discovered that gender (as measured by male and female) and race (as measured by White or Person of Color) did not impact the occurrence of ableist microaggressions; these results contrast Dávila (2015), who found that Latinx PWD encountered microaggressions that aligned with both racial and disability microaggressions.

Although scholars have provided valuable data about ableist microaggressions they have not considered the full range of sociocultural identities PWD hold or the differences in the types of ableist microaggressions (i.e., helplessness, minimization, denial of personhood, and otherization) PWD experience combined with their nondominant sociocultural identities. The purpose of this study was to examine the occurrence and types of ableist microaggressions PWD experience and whether their nondominant sociocultural identities impact their experiences of ableist microaggressions. Our intention was to reveal critical information counselors can use to better serve their clients who have disabilities and experience ableist microaggressions.

Methodology

For this quantitative study, we utilized surveys and online data collection to investigate our three research questions that aligned with the study purpose.

  • Research Question 1: What are the participants’ experiences of ableist microaggressions based on the Ableist Microaggressions Scale (AMS; Conover et al., 2017b) scores and subscale scores?
  • Research Question 2: Do AMS scores and AMS subscales (i.e., Helplessness, Minimization, Denial of Personhood, and Otherization) have a relationship with the type of disability and the visibility of the disability?
  • Research Question 3: Are other nondominant sociocultural identity factors associated with or predictive of AMS scores and AMS subscales?

We anticipated participants would report differences in the occurrence and types of ableist microaggressions they experienced based on visibility of disability and disability type and that having nondominant identities would influence the occurrence of ableist microaggressions. More specifically, we expected people with visible disabilities to report higher occurrences of ableist microaggressions than their counterparts and that disability visibility would influence the type of ableist microaggressions. We anticipated variations based on disability type regarding frequency and type of ableist microaggressions. Finally, we expected PWD nondominant sociocultural identities to be associated with the occurrence of ableist microaggressions.

Procedures
     This study was approved by the first author’s IRB. We recruited participants through targeted disability organizations (e.g., Council of State Administrators of Vocational Rehabilitation, Wisconsin’s Centers for Independent Living, Alabama Department of Rehabilitation Services) and social media platforms, many of which were specifically for PWD (e.g., Blind Pen Pals, VR Professionals, Cerebral Palsy Support, and Spinal Cord Injury Peer Mentor Program Facebook groups). Additionally, we sent email invitations to professional and personal contacts and posted on listservs of various professional organizations that serve PWD. The email invitation and posting included a brief description of the research study; inclusion criteria; an informed consent document; and a URL link to the secure, confidential survey via Qualtrics. To proceed to the study questionnaires, participants had to meet the following criteria: 1) at least 18 years old, 2) have a high school diploma or GED, and 3) have a diagnosed disability. The online survey portal was divided into six parts: informed consent, consent, screening questions, demographics, AMS questions, and two optional open-ended questions regarding their personal microaggression experiences and their interest in participating in future related studies. This study was Section 508 compliant and accessible to participants with disabilities. In total, 254 participants responded to the online survey; 53 participants were excluded because of vastly incomplete or unusable data, resulting in a final sample of 201 participants.

Data Collection
Demographic Questionnaire
     We used a self-report demographic questionnaire to collect information about age, sex, gender identity, sexual orientation, racial/ethnic identity, religion/spiritual identity, education, employment status, and source of income (see Table 1). In light of the diversity within the disability community and to allow for within-group comparisons, we asked for type of disability (i.e., physical, sensory, psychiatric, and neurodevelopmental) and visibility of disability (i.e., visible/apparent, invisible/hidden, or both). Participants reported their highest level of education and their job responsibilities as they apply to their training, education, and skills. Because income level is subjective in terms of individual needs and geographic U.S. region, and PWD often have more than one income source, we asked participants to name their sources of income instead of a dollar amount.

Ableist Microaggressions Scale
     The AMS (Conover et al., 2017b) is a 20-item self-report measure of PWD’s lifetime experiences with disability-related microaggressions. The AMS has four subscales: Helplessness, Minimization, Denial of Personhood, and Otherization. Item examples include: “People offer me unsolicited, unwanted, or unneeded help because I have a disability” (Helplessness); “People are unwilling to accept I have a disability because I appear able-bodied” (Minimization); “People don’t see me as a whole person because I have a disability” (Denial of Personhood); and “People suggest that living with a disability would not be a worthwhile existence” (Otherization). Participants were instructed to think only about their personal ableist microaggression experiences when responding to the AMS items.

Responses were rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (very frequently), and three items included a not applicable response option, as these items may not be relevant for individuals with invisible/hidden disabilities. Possible total scores range from 17 to 120, with higher scores indicating greater levels of lifetime experiences with ableist microaggressions. The overall internal consistency reliability was .92 (Conover et al., 2017a) and .95 (Conover & Israel, 2019), and the internal consistency reliability scores for the AMS subscales were .85 for Helplessness, .65 for Minimization, .90 for Denial of Personhood, and .84 for Otherization (Conover et al., 2017a). Cronbach’s alpha for the AMS in this sample was .90.

Data Analysis
     All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics Standard V27. To answer Research Question 1, we ran descriptive statistics for all measured variables of AMS scales and subscales (see Table 2). For Research Question 2, we conducted factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) and multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to understand whether the AMS scores and subscale scores differed based on the type of disability and the visibility of the disability as independent factors. To answer Research Question 3, we utilized multiple regression analyses to investigate the predictive variables of participants’ sociocultural identities and AMS scores and subscale scores.

Table 1
Demographic Characteristics of Participants (N = 201)

Variable    n   %
Disability Type a
Physical 100 49.8
Sensory   89 44.3
Psychiatric/Mental   61 30.3
Neurodevelopmental   43 21.4
Disability Type
Physical only   55 26.8
Sensory only   55 26.8
Psychiatric/Mental only   12   5.9
Neurodevelopmental only   11   5.4
Two or more disabilities   68 33.2
Disability Visibility
Visible/apparent   62 30.8
Invisible/hidden   84 41.8
Both visible and invisible   55 27.4
Age
18–29   33 16.4
30–39   44 21.9
40–49   57 28.4
50–59   39 19.4
60–69   20 10.0
70+     8   4.0
Biological Sex
Female 158 78.6
Male   43 21.4
Gender Identity
Woman 150 74.6
Man   43 21.4
Gender queer     5   2.5
Self-identify in another way     3   1.5
Affectual/Sexual Orientation
Heterosexual 155 77.1
Bisexual   20 10.0
Gay     5   2.5
Lesbian     3   1.5
Pansexual     5   2.5
Queer     7   3.5
Self-identify in another way     6   3.0
African American/Black   15   7.5
Asian or Pacific Islander American     5   2.5
Variable    n   %
Euro American/White 153 76.1
Jewish     6   3.0
Latino/a or Hispanic     8   4.0
Native American     1     .5
Middle Eastern     3   1.5
Biracial     2   1.0
Multiracial     2   1.0
Self-identify in another way     6   3.0
Religious/Spiritual Identity
Agnostic   34 16.9
Atheist   20 10.0
Buddhist     2   1.0
Christian: Catholic   32 15.9
Christian: Protestant   72 35.8
Jewish     6   3.0
Muslim     2   1.0
Self-identify in another way   33 16.4
Education
High school diploma/GED   15   7.5
Some college, but no degree   27 13.4
Associate or trade school degree   19   9.5
Bachelor’s degree   44 21.9
Master’s degree   72 35.8
PhD, EdD, JD, MD, etc.   24 11.9
Employment Status
Full time   94 46.8
Part time   30 14.9
Unemployed   34 16.9
Student   22 10.9
Retired   21 10.4
Training/Education/Skills Compared to Job Responsibilities
On par   85 42.3
Exceeding   54 26.9
Lower     4   2.0
Not applicable   58 28.9
Source of Income
Job 120 59.7
SSI   27 13.4
SSDI   37 18.4
Retirement/pension   22 10.9
Unemployment benefits     2   1.0
Other income sources   43 21.4

Note. SSI = Supplemental Security Income; SSDI = Supplemental Security Disability Income.
a Participants had the option to indicate more than one response; therefore, totals are greater than 100%.

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics for AMS Items by Subscale

    M   SD
Total AMS score 61.01 20.60
Overall AMS item   3.05   1.03
Subscale: Helplessness   3.30   1.34
H1. People feel they need to do something to help me because I have a disability.   3.29   1.61
H2. People express admiration for me or describe me as inspirational simply because I live with a disability.   3.65   1.80
H3. People express pity for me because I have a disability.   3.11   1.59
H4. People do not expect me to have a job or volunteer activities because I have a disability.   2.95   1.82
H5. People offer me unsolicited, unwanted, or unneeded help because I have a disability.   3.47   1.76
Subscale: Minimization   3.60   1.56
M6. People are unwilling to accept I have a disability because I appear able-bodied.a   3.63   2.01
M7. People minimize my disability or suggest it could be worse.   3.62   1.81
M8. People act as if accommodations for my disability are unnecessary. a   3.56   1.76
Subscale: Denial of Personhood   3.07   1.47
D9. People don’t see me as a whole person because I have a disability.   3.32   1.66
D10. People act as if I am nothing more than my disability.   2.66   1.52
D11. People speak to me as if I am a child or do not take me seriously because I have a disability.   3.16   1.74
D12. People assume I have low intelligence because I have a disability.   3.08   1.84
D13. Because I have a disability, people attempt to make decisions for me that I can make for myself.   3.11   1.70
Subscale: Otherization   2.63   1.22
O14. People think I should not date or pursue sexual relationships because I have a disability.   2.09   1.54
O15. People indicate they would not date a person with a disability.   2.93   1.73
O16. People suggest that I cannot or should not have children because I have a disability.   2.37   1.60
O17. People stare at me because I have a disability. a   3.18   1.93
O18. Because I have a disability, people seem surprised to see me outside my home.   2.42   1.65
O19. Because I have a disability, people assume I have an extraordinary gift or talent.   2.49   1.72
O20. People suggest that living with a disability would not be a worthwhile existence.   2.91   1.74

Note. AMS = Ableist Microaggressions Scale; H = Helplessness; M = Minimization; D = Denial of Personhood;
O = Otherization; M = Mean; SD = Standard Deviation
a Item includes “not applicable” response option.

 

Results

For Research Question 1, there was substantial variability in participants’ responses to individual AMS items (see Table 2). Participants had an overall total AMS mean score of 61.01 (SD = 20.60). The response mean score was 3.05 (SD = 1.03). The response means for the AMS items ranged from a high value of 3.65 (SD = 1.80) to a low value of 2.09 (SD = 1.54). In the Minimization subscale, the response mean was 3.60 (SD = 1.56) and ranged from a high value of 3.63 (SD = 2.01) for “People are unwilling to accept I have a disability because I appear able-bodied” to a low value of 3.56 (SD = 1.76) for  “People act as if accommodations for my disability are unnecessary.” For the Helplessness subscale, the response mean was 3.30 (SD = 1.34) and ranged from a high value of 3.65 (SD = 1.80) for “People express admiration for me or describe me as inspirational simply because I live with a disability” to a low value of 2.95 (SD = 1.82) for “People do not expect me to have a job or volunteer activities because I have a disability.” In the Denial of Personhood subscale (M = 3.07; SD = 1.47) the responses ranged from M = 3.32 (SD = 1.66) for “People don’t see me as a whole person because I have a disability” to M = 2.66 (SD = 1.52) for “People act as if I am nothing more than my disability.” For Otherization, the response mean was lower than the other subscales (M = 2.63; SD = 1.22) and ranged from a high value of 3.18 (SD = 1.93) for “People stare at me because I have a disability” to a low value of 2.42 (SD = 1.65) for “Because I have a disability, people seem surprised to see me outside my home.”

     For Research Question 2, we conducted a factorial ANOVA to understand the relationship between AMS scores, type of disability, and visibility of disability. Main effect results revealed that ableist microaggression experiences were significantly different for the visibility of disability factor—that is, whether the participant had visible, hidden, or both visible and hidden disabilities, F(2, 189) = 6.12, p = .003, partial ŋ2 = .061; however, ableist microaggression experiences were not significantly different based on disability type, F(4, 189) = 2.26, p = .064, partial ŋ2 = .046. The Scheffe post hoc test revealed visibility categories were significantly different. The invisible/hidden disability group significantly differed in ableist microaggression experiences from the visible/apparent disability group and the visible and hidden group. The invisible/hidden group (M = 2.57, SD = 0.11) scored significantly lower in their AMS compared with the visible/apparent disability group (M = 3.31, SD = 0.14) and visible and hidden disability group (M = 3.41, SD = 0.26). Calculated effect size revealed a small proportion of AMS variance was accounted for by visibility of disability, while interactions between type of disability were not significant, F(5, 189) = 1.69, p = .138, partial ŋ2 = .043.

We utilized a MANOVA to determine the effect of disability types and visibility of disability using four dependent variables that represented the AMS subscales (i.e., Helplessness, Minimization, Denial of Personhood, and Otherization). The Box’s Test was significant (p = .01), indicating that homogeneity of variance was not fulfilled, so we used Pillai’s trace test statistic to interpret the results. The results revealed that visibility of disability, Pillai’s V = .323, F(8, 374) = 8.99, p < .001, ŋ2 = .161, significantly affected the combination of the AMS subscales. The factor interaction indicated no statistical significance, F(20, 756) = .94, p = .535, ŋ2 = .024, nor was there statistical significance for the main effect of types of disability, Pillai’s V = .097, F(16, 756) = 1.17, p = .285, ŋ2 = .024.

The multivariate effect sizes were very small based on univariate ANOVA and Scheffe post hoc tests. ANOVA results indicated that visibility of disability significantly differed for all AMS subscales: Helplessness, F(2, 189) = 17.25, p < .001, ŋ2 = .154; Minimization, F(2, 189) = 16.02, p < .001, ŋ2 = .145; Denial of Personhood, F(2, 189) = 4.74, p = .01, ŋ2 = .048; and Otherization, F(2, 189) = 11.99, p < .001, ŋ2 = .113. Participants with visible disabilities experienced more Helplessness and Otherization microaggression types, while participants with invisible disabilities experienced more Minimization microaggressions. Participants with both visible and invisible disabilities experienced Denial of Personhood microaggressions more frequently. Table 3 presents the adjusted and unadjusted group means for AMS subscales by type of disability and visibility of disability.

Table 3
Adjusted and Unadjusted Means for AMS Subscales by Disability Types and Visibility of Disability

Subscale   Helplessness Minimization Denial of Personhood   Otherization
ADJ

M

UA

M

ADJ

M

UA

M

ADJ

M

UA

M

ADJ

M

UA

M

Disability Types
Physical only 3.09 3.38 3.00 2.80 2.66 2.85 2.45 2.69
Sensory only 3.57 3.64 3.49 3.41 3.48 3.46 2.77 2.82
Psychiatric/Mental only 2.17 2.17 4.47 4.47 2.25 2.25 1.76 1.76
Neurodevelopment only 2.95 2.25 3.70 3.73 3.05 2.11 2.77 1.88
2 or more disabilities 3.41 3.31 3.85 4.23 3.21 3.23 2.75 2.69
Visibility
Visible/apparent 4.02 4.07 2.56 2.51 3.29 3.30 3.15 3.19
Invisible/hidden 2.41 2.51 4.17 4.30 2.40 2.51 1.95 2.01
Both visible and invisible 3.54 3.63 3.59 3.76 3.64 3.65 3.01 2.93

Note. AMS = Ableist Microaggressions Scale; ADJ = Adjusted Mean; UA = Unadjusted Mean.

 

For Research Question 3, predictors were transformed and collapsed into dummy variables so they were useful for data analysis. Standard multiple regressions were conducted to determine the accuracy of the sociocultural identities (i.e., age, sex, gender identity, sexual orientation, racial/ethnic identity, religion/spiritual identity, education, and employment status) to predict AMS scores. Regression results indicated that the overall model does not significantly predict AMS scores, R2 = .052, R2adj = .019, F(14, 186) = .73, p = .74. Although the results did not predict AMS scores overall, they significantly predicted Minimization scores, R2 = .157, R2adj = .093, F(14, 186) = 2.47, p = .003. This model accounts for 9% of variance in the Minimization score. We found that race/ethnicity (β = −.51, p = .04), education level (β = −.69, p = .03), and employment status (β = 1.18, p = .03) significantly predicted AMS Minimization scores.

Discussion

Our findings both support and extend our understanding of the occurrence of ableist microaggressions and the types experienced by PWD who have different disability characteristics and who have a range of sociocultural identities. Participants in our study reported, on average, higher lifetime occurrences of ableist microaggressions (M = 3.05) than what Conover et al. (2017a) found (M = 1.70). These differences may be due, in part, to an increased recognition and awareness of ableist microaggressions. The high average scores reported in each subscale are consistent with the Helplessness, Minimization, Denial of Personhood, and Otherization scores reported by other researchers (Gonzales et al., 2015; Keller & Galgay, 2010; Olkin et al., 2019).

In our study, visibility of disability differentially impacted the overall occurrence and types of ableist microaggressions PWD experienced. Like in previous studies (Andreou et al., 2021; Conover et al., 2017a; Kattari, 2020), participants in our study with visible disabilities reported higher occurrences of ableist microaggressions than people with hidden disabilities or those with both visible and hidden disabilities, and they reported more Helplessness and Otherization types of microaggressions. It is plausible that people with visible disabilities experience Helplessness and Otherization ableist microaggressions more frequently because of the dominant culture’s perception that disability is catastrophic and results in functional limitations beyond their disability, presuming that PWD need unsolicited help from able-bodied individuals and that PWD are burdensome (Keller & Galgay, 2010; Olkin et al., 2019). Like Andreou et al. (2021) found, participants with hidden disabilities indicated fewer overall ableist microaggression experiences, but they reported higher Minimization microaggressions than their counterparts. Because people with hidden disabilities generally do not fit the stereotypical representation of a person with a disability, they may pass as nondisabled and must prove their disability status. This is consistent with findings that individuals living with concealable chronic and mental illnesses encounter experiences in which the existence or severity of their symptoms are denied by others (Gonzales et al., 2015; Olkin et al., 2019). Unique to our study is the result that participants who identified as having both visible and hidden disabilities experienced more Denial of Personhood microaggressions, indicating PWD being reduced to their disability status (Conover et al., 2017a) and/or PWD’s other sociocultural identities being ignored or denied (Keller & Galgay, 2010).

PWD with different types of disabilities did not differ in the overall occurrence of and types of ableist microaggressions they experienced. Partly, our results concur with Conover et al. (2017a), who noted no differences in the occurrence of ableist microaggressions among people with different types of physical disabilities. In contrast, Andreou et al. (2021) reported that people with medical conditions/chronic illnesses indicated fewer ableist microaggressions than their counterparts and that disability type was related to Helplessness, Minimization, and Otherization. The variability across these studies may be due to real differences or the ways in which scholars classified disability types.

The sociocultural identities we examined did not predict the lifetime experiences of ableist microaggressions, aligning with prior researchers’ results (Conover et al., 2017a). However, unique to our study is that we found that race/ethnicity, education level, and employment status are predictive of Minimization ableist microaggressions for people of color (POC), those with graduate degrees, and PWD who are employed full time. We proffer these results may be understood from the standpoint that microaggressions toward PWD are pervasive and may intersect with other nondominant identities (Sue & Spanierman, 2022). It is not surprising that participants with nondominant racial identities had higher Minimization scores given that POC frequently report being dismissed and ignored within U.S. culture (Nadal et al., 2014); based on our study results, this holds true for POC with disabilities. Additionally, because society typically views people with graduate degrees and those who work full time as more capable, PWD who fit these categories may be more likely to experience microaggressions that minimize the existence or severity of their disabilities because they are perceived as more capable based on education and employment.

Implications

Given the ubiquitous nature of ableist microaggressions experienced in the disability community, it is vital for counselors to recognize, acknowledge, validate, and be culturally aware of and sensitive to the presence of microaggressions in the lives of PWD, and in turn, consider that socioemotional problems may be a product of microaggressions rather than attributes related to their disability (Chapin et al., 2018; Sue & Spanierman, 2022). However, counselors must understand that PWD may not use the terms microaggressions, ableist, or ableism explicitly, so they need to listen and attend to client stories that communicate such experiences and determine whether or not these experiences are part of their symptomology. Grounded in the AMS domains evident in the study results, we proffer that clients may share stories that communicate instances in which PWD experience:

  • Helplessness: PWD are given unsolicited assistance, restricted in performing daily activities, denied their independence, or not directly communicated with by others.
  • Minimization: PWD are required to continuously prove, substantiate, or explain the existence of their disability.
  • Denial of Personhood: PWD must endure others’ singular focus on their disability or disregard of their additional sociocultural identities.
  • Otherization: PWD experience others denying, questioning, or expressing irritation regarding accommodation requests or must deal with people assuming that impairment
    in one area results in impairment in other areas.

Failure to appropriately attend to these inequities experienced by PWD or to engage in cultural humility can lead to early termination, impede the working alliance, and/or result in additional psychological harm (Sue & Spanierman, 2022).

Because counselors are products of their environments, they are at risk of developing unconscious biases toward PWD with visible and hidden disabilities, and left unchecked, they can unintentionally communicate these biases within the counseling process. Biased beliefs can unwittingly drive actions that can damage the counseling relationship and result in microaggressions, including seemingly well-intended, innocuous actions like holding a door (i.e., Helplessness) or unilaterally determining a treatment plan without client input (i.e., Otherization). Such actions can usurp the autonomy of clients with disabilities and result in denying clients their basic ethical rights. Additionally, counselors may inadvertently overlook disability identity when they do not include ability/disability status or questions about disability or chronic illness as part of their intake and assessment procedures (i.e., Denial of Personhood; Cook et al., 2020). Without this knowledge, they may mistakenly minimize a client’s hidden, undisclosed disability because they were unaware of it (i.e., Minimization), yet our results support that this may occur with apparent/disclosed disabilities, too. Consequently, we recommend counselors provide intake questions that give clients the opportunity to identify their disabilities, to include additional self-determined relevant information about their disability, and to express how they would like the counselor to refer to their disability. Furthermore, counselors must follow up about intake form information during the clinical interview (Cook et al., 2020).

PWD with multiple intersecting nondominant sociocultural identities experience the inherent complexities associated with possessing overt cultural identities (e.g., POC) and concealed cultural identities (e.g., sexual/affectual orientation). It is essential for counselors to explore clients’ identities that are most important to them (i.e., identity salience; Hunt et al., 2006) and their experiences of privilege and marginalization. Accordingly, counselors must work to understand the privileged and marginalized statuses related to all of their identities and specifically related to ability/disability in order to broach effectively. To do so, counselors must examine their attitudes, beliefs, and assumptions about PWD to combat unconscious biases that could influence their behavior or interpretations in the counseling relationship (Chapin et al., 2018). To do so, Deroche et al. (2020) recommended reading autobiographies or blogs written by PWD (e.g., Heumann & Joiner, 2021), listening to or watching Ted Talks given by PWD (e.g., Young, 2014), following or joining social media pages specific to disability or PWD, and/or reading scholarly literature centered on the lives of PWD. Additionally, we recommend counselors engage in culturally responsive care, including use of disability-inclusive language (Kattari, 2020); discuss specific accessibility and accommodation needs (Chapin et al., 2018); assess office accessibility; and create disability-friendly policies (Chapin et al., 2018; Olkin et al., 2019).

Counselor educators and supervisors are responsible for preparing students and supervisees to work with PWD. Consequently, professional counselors need more than one day slated for disability topics in their multicultural counseling course. Most importantly, professional counselors need to know that PWD have historically been and are continually on the frontlines advocating for their civil rights and promoting social justice and equality. Although it is likely impossible for most programs to add another course to their curriculum, it is reasonable and doable to integrate disability into established courses as they have for other cultural topics and for supervisors to make intentional efforts to address ability/disability within clinical supervision. Strategies include creating case studies that portray PWD or disability identity (see Smart, 2012 for examples); developing activities, assignments, or projects that require counselor trainees to explore negative attitudinal barriers and social inequities experienced by PWD (Deroche et al., 2020); discussing how disability impacts family roles, responsibilities, and dynamics; using cultural broaching in the classroom and in supervision (Day-Vines et al., 2021); and designing program policies and materials that reduce attitudinal and access-related barriers for students and supervisees with disabilities.

Limitations and Future Research

The study results must be understood in the context of its limitations. Self-selection bias may have influenced who decided to participate in the study. Although we included a definition and examples of ableist microaggressions in our informed consent document, PWD who were more familiar with or had an interest in this topic may have chosen to participate compared to PWD who were less familiar or knowledgeable about ableist microaggressions. Our use of the AMS (Conover et al., 2017b) introduces limitations associated with its psychometric properties, as no normative data is available. Additionally, the AMS purports to measure the lifetime occurrence of ableist microaggressions, requiring participants to retrospectively report information from years prior; retrospective recall of ableist microaggressions may result in inaccurate data (Kattari, 2019). Although there is benefit to understanding the long-term effects of ableist microaggressions, we suggest that researchers pursue longitudinal studies rather than utilizing a one-time measure that relies on participants’ recollections.

Although our sample included people with a range of disability types and visibility of disability, the majority identified as White, heterosexual women who are employed and who had some level of higher education experience. The racial (Goyat et al., 2016), educational (Paul et al., 2021), and employment (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2023) inequities reported in the disability community are not representative of our sample. We suggest that future studies intentionally sample individuals who are more representative of the disability community. Finally, counseling researchers must continue to investigate how ableist microaggressions manifest in the counseling relationship, best practices for helping clients heal from microaggressive experiences, and appropriate supervision and educational interventions to prepare counselors and counselors-in-training to work with PWD who have experienced ableist microaggressions.

Conclusion

The results of our study add to the paucity of research specific to ableist microaggressions, particularly with respect to visibility of disability and other nondominant sociocultural identities of PWD. Like other studies, we found high average scores in all domains, and that visibility of disability resulted in higher AMS scores and impacted the type of ableist microaggressions experienced. Although the sociocultural identities we examined did not predict the lifetime experiences of ableist microaggressions, we found race/ethnicity, education level, and employment status are predictive of Minimization ableist microaggressions for POC, those with graduate degrees, and PWD who are employed full time. We offered that all counseling professionals—counselors, counselors-in-training, counselor educators, and supervisors—must attend to disability microaggressions regularly within their professional roles and specifically, we urged researchers to increase their attention to ableist microaggression research. Together, we can reduce ableist microaggressions and eventually increase access for PWD within professional counseling.

 

Conflict of Interest and Funding Disclosure
The authors reported no conflict of interest
or funding contributions for the development
of this manuscript.

 

References

Andreou, E., Psyllou, A., Vlachou, A., Fyssa, A., & Saridaki, M. (2021). Microaggressions and psychosocial adjustment among Greek university students with disabilities. Education Sciences, 11(12), 781–799. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11120781

Capodilupo, C. M., Nadal, K. L., Corman, L., Hamit, S., Lyons, O. B., & Weinberg, A. (2010). The manifestation of gender microaggressions. In D. W. Sue (Ed.), Microaggressions and marginality: Manifestation, dynamics, and impact (pp. 193–216). Wiley.

Chapin, M., McCarthy, H., Shaw, L., Bradham-Cousar, M., Chapman, R., Nosek, M., Peterson, S., Yilmaz, Z., & Ysasi, N. (2018). Disability-related counseling competencies. American Rehabilitation Counseling Association Task Force on Competencies for Counseling Persons with Disabilities. https://bit.ly/ARCA2018

Conover, K. J., & Israel, T. (2019). Microaggressions and social support among sexual minorities with physical disabilities. Rehabilitation Psychology, 64(2), 167–178. https://doi.org/10.1037/rep0000250

Conover, K. J., Israel, T., & Nylund-Gibson, K. (2017a). Development and validation of the Ableist Microaggressions Scale. The Counseling Psychologist, 45(4), 570–599.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000017715317

Conover, K. J., Israel, T., & Nylund-Gibson, K. (2017b). Ableist Microaggressions Scale [Database record]. APA PsycTESTS. https://doi.org/10.1037/t64519-000

Cook, J. M., Skaistis, S. M., Borden, S., & Nair, D. (2020). Inquiring about client cultural identities: A content analysis of intake paperwork. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 42(3), 220–233. https://doi.org/10.17744/mehc.42.3.03

Dávila, B. (2015). Critical race theory, disability microaggressions, and Latina/o student experiences in special education. Race, Ethnicity, and Education, 18(4), 443–468. https://doi.org/10.1080/13613324.2014.885422

Day-Vines, N. L., Cluxton-Keller, F., Agorsor, C., & Gubara, S. (2021). Strategies for broaching the subjects of race, ethnicity, and culture. Journal of Counseling & Development, 99(3), 348–357. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcad.12380

Deroche, M. D., Herlihy, B., & Lyons, M. L. (2020). Counselor trainee self-perceived disability competence: Implications for training. Counselor Education and Supervision, 59(3), 187–199. https://doi.org/10.1002/ceas.12183

Eisenman, L. T., Rolón-Dow, R., Freedman, B., Davison, A., & Yates, N. (2020). “Disabled or not, people just want to feel welcome”: Stories of microaggressions and microaffirmations from college students with intellectual disability. Critical Education, 11(17), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.14288/ce.v11i17.186499

Forrest-Bank, S. S., & Cuellar, M. J. (2018). The mediating effects of ethnic identity on the relationship between racial microaggression and psychological well-being. Social Work Research, 42(1), 44–56.
https://doi.org/10.1093/swr/svx023

Gonzales, L., Davidoff, K. C., Nadal, K. L., & Yanos, P. T. (2015). Microaggressions experienced by persons with mental illnesses: An exploratory study. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 38(3), 234–241.
https://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000096

Goyat, R., Vyas, A., & Sambamoorthi, U. (2016). Racial/ethnic disparities in disability prevalence. Journal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities, 3(4), 635–645. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40615-015-0182-z

Heumann, J., & Joiner, K. (2021). Being Heumann: An unrepentant memoir of a disability rights activist. Penguin Random House.

Hunt, B., Matthews, C., Milsom, A., & Lammel, J. A. (2006). Lesbians with physical disabilities: A qualitative study of their experiences with counseling. Journal of Counseling & Development, 84(2), 163–173.
https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6678.2006.tb00392.x

Kattari, S. K. (2019). The development and validation of the Ableist Microaggression Inventory. Journal of Social Service Research, 45(3), 400–417. https://doi.org/10.1080/01488376.2018.1480565

Kattari, S. K. (2020). Ableist microaggressions and the mental health of disabled adults. Community Mental Health Journal, 56(6), 1170–1179. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10597-020-00615-6

Keller, R. M., & Galgay, C. E. (2010). Microaggressive experiences of people with disabilities. In D. W. Sue (Ed.), Microaggressions and marginality: Manifestation, dynamics, and impact (pp. 241–268). Wiley.

Lee, E.-J., Ditchman, N., Thomas, J., & Tsen, J. (2019). Microaggressions experienced by people with multiple sclerosis in the workplace: An exploratory study using Sue’s taxonomy. Rehabilitation Psychology, 64(2), 179–193. https://doi.org/10.1037/rep0000269

Lett, K., Tamaian, A., & Klest, B. (2020). Impact of ableist microaggressions on university students with self-identified disabilities. Disability & Society, 35(9), 1441–1456. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687599.2019.1680344

Nadal, K. L., Griffin, K. E., Wong, Y., Hamit, S., & Rasmus, M. (2014). The impact of racial microaggressions on mental health: Counseling implications for clients of color. Journal of Counseling & Development, 92(1), 57–66. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.2014.00130.x

Olkin, R., Hayward, H., Abbene, M. S., & VanHeel, G. R. (2019). The experiences of microaggressions against women with visible and invisible disabilities. Journal of Social Issues, 75(3), 757–785. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12342

Paul, S., Rafal, M., & Houtenville, A. (2021). 2021 annual disability statistics compendium. University of New Hampshire, Institute on Disability. https://disabilitycompendium.org/compendium/2021-annual-disability-statistics-compendium-a

Smart, J. (2012). Disability across the developmental life span: An introduction for the helping professions (2nd ed.). Springer.

Sue, D. W., & Spanierman, L. B. (2022). Microaggressions in everyday life (2nd ed.). Wiley.

U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2023, February 23). Persons with a disability: Labor force characteristics summary. https://www.bls.gov/news.release/disabl.nr0.htm

Young, S. (2014). I’m not your inspiration, thank you very much [Video]. https://bit.ly/YoungNotyourinspiration

Melissa D. Deroche, PhD, NCC, ACS, LPC-S, is an assistant professor at Tarleton State University. Lee Za Ong, PhD, LPC, CRC, is an assistant professor at Marquette University. Jennifer M. Cook, PhD, NCC, ACS, LPC, is an associate professor at the University of Texas at San Antonio. Correspondence may be addressed to Melissa D. Deroche, Tarleton State University, Department of Counseling, 10850 Texan Rider Dr., Ft. Worth, TX 76036, mderoche@tarleton.edu.